Hebrew U. researchers gain insight into brain mechanisms underlying dyslexia

Dyslexia is believed to affect one in every 10 to 20 people, harming their ability to read and spell words, but not affecting their general intelligence.

By
January 25, 2017 23:42
2 minute read.
The national library of Israel

The national library of Israel on the Givat Ram campus, the Hebrew University of Jerusalem.. (photo credit: Courtesy)

 
X

Dear Reader,
As you can imagine, more people are reading The Jerusalem Post than ever before. Nevertheless, traditional business models are no longer sustainable and high-quality publications, like ours, are being forced to look for new ways to keep going. Unlike many other news organizations, we have not put up a paywall. We want to keep our journalism open and accessible and be able to keep providing you with news and analyses from the frontlines of Israel, the Middle East and the Jewish World.

As one of our loyal readers, we ask you to be our partner.

For $5 a month you will receive access to the following:

  • A user experience almost completely free of ads
  • Access to our Premium Section
  • Content from the award-winning Jerusalem Report and our monthly magazine to learn Hebrew - Ivrit
  • A brand new ePaper featuring the daily newspaper as it appears in print in Israel

Help us grow and continue telling Israel’s story to the world.

Thank you,

Ronit Hasin-Hochman, CEO, Jerusalem Post Group
Yaakov Katz, Editor-in-Chief

UPGRADE YOUR JPOST EXPERIENCE FOR 5$ PER MONTH Show me later

The common learning difficulty of dyslexia may be diagnosed, and treatment offered at an early stage, as the result of a new discovery at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

Humans have a type of long-term memory (called “implicit memory”) in which we respond less to stimuli as they are repeated over time (called “adaptation”). Now, Jerusalem researchers reveal that dyslexics recover faster than non-dyslexics from their responses to stimuli, such as sounds and written words, leading to their perceptual and reading difficulties.

Be the first to know - Join our Facebook page.


Dyslexia is believed to affect one in every 10 to 20 people, harming their ability to read and spell words, but not affecting their general intelligence. In a new study published in the peer-reviewed journal eLife, Prof. Merav Ahissar of the psychology department and the Edmond & Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences and her team carried out a number of experiments with dyslexics and non-dyslexics, to shine new light on the brain mechanisms underlying this condition.

“While dyslexics are diagnosed mainly according to their reading difficulty, they also differ from non-dyslexics in performing simple perceptual tasks, such as tone-frequency discrimination,” said first author Sagi Jaffe-Dax.

Ahissar explained: “Our lab previously found that this is due to ‘poor anchoring,’ where dyslexics have an inefficient integration of information from recent stimuli, collected as implicit memory. This memory typically forms ‘anchors’ that provide specific predictions that perceptually clarify noisy stimuli, and we wanted to see why this is not the case in dyslexics.”

In the current study, the team gave 60 native Hebrew speakers, including 30 dyslexics and 30 non-dyslexics, frequency discrimination and oral reading tasks. During the frequency discrimination task, participants were asked to compare two tones in each trial. All participants’ responses were affected – or biased – by implicit memory of previous stimuli. Both groups were affected in similar ways by very recent stimuli, but dyslexics were less affected by earlier stimuli.

“This suggests that implicit memory decays faster among dyslexics,” said Jaffe-Dax. “We decided to test this hypothesis by increasing the length of time between consecutive stimuli and measuring how it affects behavioral biases and neural responses from the auditory cortex, a section of the brain that processes sound. Participants with dyslexia showed a faster decay of implicit memory on both measures. This also affected their oral reading rate, which decreased faster as a result of the time interval between reading the same non-word – a group of letters that looks or sounds like a word – numerous times.”

JPOST VIDEOS THAT MIGHT INTEREST YOU:


The team concluded that dyslexics’ faster recovery from stimuli can account for their longer reading times, as it causes less reliable predictions for both simple and complex stimuli.

Coauthor Orr Frenkel said: “The formation of adequate predictions is crucial for becoming an expert in general, and an expert reader in particular.

Achieving this depends on matching printed words with predictions based on previous encounters with related words. These predictions are less accurate in dyslexics.”

Join Jerusalem Post Premium Plus now for just $5 and upgrade your experience with an ads-free website and exclusive content. Click here>>

Related Content

‘THE TORAH, with its myriad laws and directives, never once explicitly requires a person to be tranq
October 17, 2018
Be. Here. Now. An introduction to Jewish mindfulness

By BEN EPSTEIN